Religious openness hypothesis: I. religious reflection, schemas, and orientations within religious fundamentalist and biblical foundationalist ideological surrounds.
Watson, P.J. ; Chen, Zhuo ; Ghorbani, Nima 等
Central to the Ideological Surround Model (ISM) of psychology and
religion is the postmodern claim that religions and social sciences
operate as incommensurable social rationalities (Watson, 1993, 2011,
2014; Ghorbani, Watson, Saeedi, Chen, & Silver, 2012). Social
rationalities are incommensurable when communities bring their thought
and practice into conformity with different ultimate standards
(MacIntyre, 1988). In Christianity and other traditional religions, the
ultimate standard will be some community-specific vision of God. In the
social sciences, the ultimate standard will be some at least implicitly
shared reading of nature.
Assertions based upon such different standards will sometimes, but
not always, be incompatible. On other occasions, they will be
compatible; and quite often, they will simply be irrelevant to each
other. Unavailable outside these "supernatural" and
"natural" rationalities, however, will be a fully objective,
standard-independent rationality for judging standards, a fact which
makes them "incommensurable" by definition.
Incommensurable rationalities reveal the important role of
ideology. MacIntyre (1978) defines ideologies as somewhat non-empirical,
normative, and sociological systems of belief. Incommensurable
rationalities are somewhat non-empirical because they rest upon faith in
some ultimate standard that can help organize but cannot be proven by
empirical observations. Research findings of an evolutionary
psychologist, for example, will not convince a Christian psychologist of
the non-existence of God nor of the ultimacy of nature. Conversely, the
social scientific work of a Christian psychologist will not demonstrate
to the evolutionary psychologist that nature must be understood under
the higher standard of God. The evolutionary psychologist will instead
believe that "God" will have a fully natural explanation.
These and other standards within contemporary social life give rise to
norms that guide the thought and practice of a vast array of
sociologically distinct communities. The broader implications of these
somewhat non-empirical, normative, and sociological systems of thought
should, therefore, be clear. Social rationalities necessarily operate
within an ideological surround.
Given the challenges of diversity within pluralistic culture, the
ISM pursues methodological innovations that seek to bring the ultimate
standards of incommensurable rationalities into sharper focus. This
effort assumes that between the social science of populations and the
social science of individuals there must be a social science of
communities. Nomothetic and ideographic research procedures clarify
populations and individuals, respectively. "Ideologographic"
approaches are necessary to illuminate communities (Watson, 2011). Among
these ideologographic procedures is the use of statistical procedures to
control for the influence of ideology. In one project, for instance,
partial correlations controlling for anti-Christian humanistic and
anti-humanistic Christian language within psychological scales made it
possible to better understand both Christian and humanistic ideological
surrounds (Watson, Morris, & Hood, 1987). The present project used
statistical controls for ideology to highlight important diversities
within the communal rationalities of Christians.
Research Into Religious Openness
Among other things, the ISM assumes that incommensurable
rationalities mean that the definition of psychological processes can
vary with commitments to different ultimate standards. Religious
rationalities, for example, may include definitions of psychological and
religious openness that are in conformity with their own, but not
necessarily with social scientific standards (Kamble, Watson,
Marigoudar, & Chen, 2014b; also see Hood, Hill, & Williamson,
2005).
Formal development of this claim emerged out of research into
religious motivation. As initially conceptualized, the Intrinsic
Religious Orientation Scale records an adaptive attempt of individuals
to sincerely live their faith, whereas the Extrinsic Religious
Orientation Scale assesses an often more maladaptive use of religion to
accomplish other ends (Allport & Ross, 1967). Research has generally
confirmed the adjustment expectations for these two measures (Donahue,
1985). Strong relationships with conservative religiosity, nevertheless,
led to a skeptical reinterpretation of the Intrinsic Scale as an index
of cognitive and religious rigidity that often predicts adjustment
merely out of social desirability concerns (Batson, Schoenrade, &
Ventis, 1993). A Quest Scale sought to operationalize a more truly open
religious motivation in which "religion involves an open-ended,
responsive dialogue with existential questions raised by the
contradictions and tragedies of life" (Batson et al., p. 169).
Some items from the Quest Scale highlight doubt as evidence of
religious openness, a fact that led Dover, Miner, and Dowson (2007) to
evaluate this instrument as inappropriate for use with Muslims. They
argued that for Muslims, openness necessarily "operates within a
faith tradition, and for the purpose of finding religious truth"
(p. 204). In other words, Quest essentially reflects an
extra-traditional definition of openness associated with the ideological
surround of an incommensurable social scientific rationality. These
researchers used Australian and Malaysian samples to devise an
intra-traditional Islamic Religious Reflection Scale for
operationalizing an explicitly Muslim form of openness.
A later American study modified the language of this instrument to
make it appropriate for Christians. This Christian Religious Reflection
Scale turned out to have Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection factors
that correlated negatively (Watson, Chen, & Hood, 2011). Faith
Oriented Reflection recorded a Christian-centered approach to
understanding that appeared in such self-reports as, "Faith in
Christ is what nourishes the intellect and makes the intellectual life
prosperous and productive." Intellect Oriented Reflection assessed
openness to forms of understanding that were not specific to Christian
commitment. One item said, for instance, "I believe as humans we
should use our minds to explore all fields of thought from science to
metaphysics." Faith Oriented Reflection predicted higher Intrinsic
and lower Quest scores, whereas Intellect Oriented Reflection displayed
an opposite pattern of results.
This American study also used statistical controls for ideology in
order to differentiate between Religious Fundamentalist and Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds. The Altemeyer and Hunsberger
(1992) Religious Fundamentalism Scale records beliefs "there is one
set of religious teachings that clearly contains the fundamental, basic,
intrinsic, essential, inerrant truth about humanity and deity; that this
essential truth is fundamentally opposed by forces of evil which must be
vigorously fought; that this truth must be followed today according to
the fundamental, unchangeable practices of the past; and that those who
believe and follow these fundamental teachings have a special
relationship with the deity" (p. 118). In an earlier project, ISM
ideologographic procedures had "translated" statements from
this instrument into a Biblical Foundationalist language that was less
aggressive, more thoughtful, and more sensitive to non-fundamentalist
perspectives (Watson et al., 2003). Partial correlations controlling for
the Religious Fundamentalism Scale revealed that the two Religious
Reflection factors could co-vary directly in American Christian samples
and that the less defensive Biblical Foundationalism could be compatible
with Intellect as well as with Faith Oriented Reflection.
In America, the negative zero-order correlation between Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection suggested a polarization in religious
thinking that had not been explored as a possibility in Muslim samples
(Dover et al., 2007). A study in Iran, therefore, reexamined the Islamic
Religious Reflection Scale and its two factors using samples of
university students from Tehran and Islamic seminarians from Qom
(Ghorbani, Watson, Chen, & Dover, 2013). Most important were
observations that Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated
positively rather than negatively in Iran and that both predicted
greater openness. The two forms of Muslim religious reflection also
displayed a direct association with the Intrinsic Scale, and Faith
Oriented Reflection correlated negatively whereas Intellect Oriented
Reflection correlated nonsignificantly with Quest.
A further analysis of religious rationalities modified the language
of the Dover et al. (2007) instrument in order to create a Hindu
Religious Reflection Scale. Graduate students in India responded to this
measure. Faith and Intellect Oriented Religious Reflection once again
displayed direct relationships with each other and with measures of
religious and psychological openness (Kamble et al., 2014b). Both
factors also predicted higher scores on the Intrinsic Religious
Orientation Scale, and Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated
positively and Faith Oriented Reflection correlated nonsignificantly
with Quest.
In summary, Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated
negatively in American Christians, but positively in Iranian Muslims and
Indian Hindus. Religious reflection, therefore, was more polarized in
the United States. Further evidence of polarization appeared when Faith
Oriented Reflection correlated positively and Intellect Oriented
Reflection correlated negatively with the Intrinsic Orientation in the
United States, whereas both forms of religious reflection displayed a
direct relationship with the Intrinsic Orientation in Iran and India. In
India, Faith Oriented Reflection also correlated nonsignificantly with
Quest, as did Intellect Oriented Reflection in Iran. In the United
States, however, Faith Oriented Reflection correlated negatively and
Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated positively with Quest. In other
words, American Christians seemed less able to integrate intellect with
faith in a manner that could make the extra-traditional Quest definition
of openness more irrelevant to their religious reflection.
Religious Openness Hypothesis
In response to these data, the ISM proposes a Religious Openness
Hypothesis which argues that positive linkages between Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection reveal that religious traditions include
standard-specific definitions of openness that can unify intellect with
faith (Kamble et al., 2014b). In addition to findings for Muslims in
Iran and Hindus in India, a direct relationship between these two forms
of religious reflection after controlling for the Religious
Fundamentalism Scale confirms the same potential in Bible-believing
Americans. The negative zero-order correlation between Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection, therefore, suggests that aspects of
fundamentalism obscure American religious openness (Watson et al.,
2011).
The Religious Openness Hypothesis explains this obscuring influence
by suggesting that conservative Christian perspectives in the United
States can include a defensive ghettoization of faith in response to a
perceived inhospitality of Western secularization and its emphasis on
reason as a replacement for belief in God in the organization of social
life (e.g., Stout, 1988). The negative correlation between Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection empirically defines this ghettoization.
This relationship, in other words, reveals a faith that walls out the
intellect and retreats into a reflective security that refuses to
consider practices associated with any standard but its own.
In more theoretical terms, the ISM contrasts ghettoization with
actualization (Watson, 2011). Actualization occurs when a community
re-enacts its traditions using innovations that allow it to faithfully
explain and behaviorally manifest itself within an increasingly complex
pluralistic culture. Actualization presupposes that practices developed
out of extra-traditional standards can have an innovative potential that
does not require any actual embrace of those outside standards
themselves. Christian uses of at least some conceptual frameworks and
the empirical methods of contemporary psychology illustrate the
possibility. Ghettoization instead follows from the opposite belief that
faithful re-enactment of traditions requires a rejection of innovation.
The result is a defensive walling out of developments outside the
community.
The ISM further assumes that a more viable transmission of
tradition across generations will likely occur with actualization than
with ghettoization (cf., Ghorbani et al., 2012). This would be so
because adoption of extra-traditional practices could promote a more
sociologically expansive translation of the intra-traditional standard.
A more expansive translation could then strengthen faith within the
community by helping tradition speak to the realities of changing
Christian experience within a pluralistic culture. Such a translation
might also enhance the plausibility of Christian standards for those
living outside the tradition. Such individuals would include new
generations of children who are born into the confusions of pluralistic
cultural life and adults who struggle in their attempts to follow other
standards. In other words, appropriately translated extra-traditional
practices could supply a bridge for such individuals to discover
openings toward incommensurable Christian rationalities.
Opposite stances by Christians on innovation reflect deeper
conflicts that point toward the further ISM assumption that
incommensurable rationalities can occur not just between a religion and
other communities, but also within a single religion (Watson, 2014).
Christians unite behind the standard of Christ; but interpretations of
that standard can emerge from very different epistemological
perspectives. Different epistemological perspectives can then cause
Christians to calibrate their thought and practices to importantly
different visions of the standard. Contrasts between Religious
Fundamentalist and Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds
illustrate the possibility. In Americans, the Religious Fundamentalism
Scale theoretically reflects a religious rationality that combines
defensiveness to secularization with a commitment to
"fundamentals." Biblical Foundationalism instead represents
the incommensurable rationality of a commitment to
"fundamentals" without defensiveness. Again, incommensurable
does not necessarily mean incompatible; and these two scales do display
a robust positive correlation (Watson et al., 2003). Statistical
controls for ideology, nevertheless, confirm Biblical Foundationalism as
a less and Religious Fundamentalism as a more defensive Christian
ideological surround (Watson et al., 2003, 2011; Watson, Chen, &
Morris, 2014).
Present Project
The present project further examined the openness of the Religious
Fundamentalist and Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds in
American Christians. In addition to assessing Faith and Intellect
Oriented Religious Reflection, procedures administered the Religious
Schema Scale (Streib, Hood, & Klein, 2010) and Religious Orientation
instruments that included a recently developed Extrinsic Cultural
Religious Orientation measure (Ghorbani, Watson, Zarehi, &
Shamohammadi, 2010; Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014). An attempt to
evaluate cognitive openness involved use of the Need for Cognition Scale
(Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, 1996).
The Religious Schema Scale assesses different styles of
interpreting experience that range from closed fundamentalism to open
tolerance. The Truth of Texts and Teachings subscale assesses a
fundamentalist style that correlates negatively with tolerance in the
West, but can also predict greater openness in India and thus has a
potential to record a more non-defensive form of fundamentalism (Kamble
et al., 2014b). Two other subscales operationalize religious openness.
Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality records "a religious style in
which openness for fairness and tolerance stands in the
foreground." Another subscale assesses "xenosophia,"
which in terms of Greek origins of the word refers to the foreigner
(xeno) and to wisdom (sophia). Xenosophia, therefore, reflects the
wisdom of "a religious style which is characterized by an
appreciation of the alien and thus by interreligious dialog"
(Streib et al., p. 167).
Administration of religious orientation scales made it possible to
evaluate the religious motivational implications of all other variables.
Quest (Batson & Schoenrade, 1991a, b) operationalized an
extra-traditional, social scientific understanding of religious
openness. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Orientation Scales (Gorsuch &
McPherson, 1989) record more intra-traditional forms of commitment. The
Extrinsic Scale includes an Extrinsic Personal factor that involves the
use of religion to achieve personal well-being and an Extrinsic Social
factor in which religion serves as a means for obtaining desired social
outcomes. Studies in Iran and Pakistan (Ghorbani, Watson, & Khan,
2007), India (Kamble, Watson, Marigoudar, & Chen, 2014a), and the
United States (Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014) suggest that the
Extrinsic Personal Orientation is largely adaptive, but the Extrinsic
Social Orientation is relatively weak and exhibits ambiguous
associations with other variables. Extrinsic Social data, therefore,
suggest that this construct largely fails to assess the important social
contributions that believers presumably attribute to their religious
motivations. Hence, the Extrinsic Cultural Religious Orientation Scale
operationalizes motivations to use religion to benefit society (Ghorbani
et al., 2010; Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014) and includes Family
and Social Order, Disorder Avoidance, Peace and Justice, and Cultural
Foundations subscales.
Hypotheses
In summary, the Religious Openness Hypothesis argues that religious
communities pursue truth with an openness that is compatible with
intra-traditional standards. This pursuit reflects the thought and
practices of an incommensurable rationality that may not always be
compatible with extra-traditional social scientific standards. Positive
correlations between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection document
the potential for religious openness in Iran and India. A negative
correlation between these two constructs in the United States
theoretically reflects a fundamentalist defensiveness that is not
evident within a Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround. The
present project used five groups of measures to evaluate this
description of American Christian religious openness.
First, and most importantly, Faith and Intellect Oriented Christian
Reflection Scales made it possible to focus on the negative correlation
between these two measures that serves as an empirical marker of
ghettoization in the United States.
Second, Religious Fundamentalism and Biblical Foundationalism
scales made it possible to analyze what the ISM presumes to be
incommensurable Christian rationalities. Analysis of a Religious
Fundamentalist Ideological Surround involved statistical procedures that
partialed out variance associated with Biblical Foundationalism, whereas
examination of a Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround
controlled for Religious Fundamentalism. Religious openness should be
less obvious within Religious Fundamentalist and more evident within
Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds.
Third, Religious Schema Scales made it possible to explore
religious styles that ranged from closed fundamentalism to open
religious tolerance. The Truth of Texts and Teaching subscale may assess
a more non-defensive form of fundamentalism. Fairness, Tolerance, and
Rationality and Xenosophia record tolerant openness.
Fourth, Religious Orientation scales made it possible to evaluate
the religious motivational implications of the Religious Fundamentalist
and Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds. Intrinsic and
various extrinsic scales examined intra-traditional forms of commitment,
whereas the Quest Scale pointed toward a more extra-traditional standard
of openness.
Fifth and finally, administration of the Need for Cognition Scale
made it possible to evaluate the cognitive openness of these two
American ideological surrounds.
Use of these measures made it possible to test two most important
sets of hypotheses:
First, as the index of a more defensive form of Christian
commitment, partial correlations for Religious Fundamentalism should be
positive with Faith Oriented Reflection; with Truth of Texts and
Teachings; with Intrinsic, Extrinsic Personal, and perhaps Extrinsic
Social Religious Orientations; and with all four Extrinsic Cultural
Religious Orientations. They should also be negative with Intellect
Oriented Reflection; Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality; Xenosophia;
Quest; and Need for Cognition.
Second, as the index of a more non-defensive form of Christian
commitment, partial correlations for Biblical Foundationalism should be
positive with both forms of Religious Reflection; all three Religious
Schema measures; Intrinsic, Extrinsic Personal, and perhaps Extrinsic
Social Religious Orientations; all four Extrinsic Cultural motivations;
and Need for Cognition. The further expectation was for either a
negative or nonsignificant relationship with a Quest measure that is
either incompatible with or irrelevant to the standards of this
Christian ideological surround.
Method
Participants
Research participants were undergraduates enrolled in Introductory
Psychology classes at a state university in the southeastern United
States. This group included 116 men, 232 women, and 2 individuals who
failed to indicate their gender. Average age was 18.4 (SD = 1.4). The
sample was 85.7% White, 8.7% African-American, and 5.6% various other
racial self-identifications. Self-reported religious affiliations were
40.3% Protestant, 11.5% Catholic, 6.9% atheist or agnostic, and the
remaining 41.3% self-categorized as "Other." Subsequent
investigations revealed that this surprisingly high "Other"
percentage was overwhelming explained by Protestants who failed to
understand these category distinctions, which were used in the present
project for the first time at this particular university. As in most
previous and subsequent investigations using similar samples, the
percentage of Protestants was likely around 75%.
Measures
Scales appeared in a single questionnaire booklet. Responses to all
items ranged across a 5-point strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree
(4) Likert scale. Instruments appeared within the booklet in the order
in which they are described below.
Need for Cognition. Eighteen statements made up the Cacioppo et al.
(1996) Need for Cognition Scale (M response per item = 2.12, SD = 0.60,
[alpha] = .85). Illustrating this measure was the self-report, "I
really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to
problems."
Religious Schema. The three Religious Schema measures included 5
items each (Streib et al., 2010). Texts and Teachings (M = 2.62, SD =
1.04, [alpha] = .85) appeared in such beliefs as, "What the texts
and stories of my religion tell me is absolutely true and must not be
changed." Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality (M = 3.17, SD =
0.56, [alpha] = .62) included, for example, the claim, "When I make
a decision, I look at all sides of the issue and come up with the best
decision possible." A representative expression of Xenosophia (M =
2.20, SD = 0.73, [alpha] = .62) asserted, "It is important to
understand others through a sympathetic understanding of their culture
and religion."
Quest. The Quest Scale of Batson and
Schoenrade (1991a, b) included 12 items (M = 1.74, SD = 0.63,
[alpha] = .74). Illustrating Quest was the claim that "I am
constantly questioning my religious beliefs."
Extrinsic Cultural Religious Orientation.
Included in the Extrinsic Cultural Religious Orientation Scale were
32 total items (Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014). Sixteen statements
operationalized Family and Social Order (M = 1.77, SD = 0.95, [alpha] =
.95) and appeared in such beliefs as, "Religious life is important
because it promotes better family relationships." The Disorder
Avoidance subscale (M = 1.71, SD = 0.89, [alpha] = .76) contained 5
items (e.g., "Most of the problems of society result from the
failure of people to be sincerely religious"). Exemplifying the
5-item Peace and Justice subscale (M = 2.03, SD = 0.80, [alpha] = .75)
was the statement, "My motivation for being religious is a desire
to develop a human society that is peaceful, just, and happy."
Representative of the 6 Cultural Foundations items (M = 1.85, SD = 0.86,
[alpha] = .78) was the self-report, "I am religious because I know
that the loss of religious life leads to the decline of civilization and
culture."
Religious Orientations. Gorsuch and McPherson (1989) scales
assessed Intrinsic and Extrinsic Religious Orientations. The Intrinsic
Scale (M = 2.56, SD = 0.88, [alpha] = .84) included 8 items which said,
for instance, "My whole approach to life is based on my
religion." Illustrating the 3-item Extrinsic Personal Orientation
(M = 2.33, SD = 0.97, [alpha] = .71) was the self-report, "What
religion offers me most is comfort in times of trouble and sorrow."
The Extrinsic Social Orientation (M = 1.17, SD = 0.88, [alpha] = .72)
also included 3 items (e.g., "I go to church mostly to spend time
with my friends"). As noted in the introduction and as will be
discussed more fully in a companion project to this investigation
(Watson, Ghorbani, Vartanian, & Chen, 2015), the relative strength
of these three orientations is a noteworthy issue. Statistical
procedures, therefore, analyzed the means of these three measures in
preparation for the companion project. Significant differences appeared,
Greenhouse-Geisser F [1.90, 662.85] = 279 79, p < .001. All post hoc
comparisons were statistically significant, with the Intrinsic
Orientation highest, the Extrinsic Personal Orientation intermediate,
and the Extrinsic Social Orientation lowest.
Christian Religious Reflection. The Christian Religious Reflection
Scale included 12 statements (Watson et al., 2011). Seven items
expressed Faith Oriented Reflection (M = 2.49, SD = 0.83, [alpha] = .80)
with the remaining 5 statements recording Intellect Oriented Reflection
(M = 2.45, SD = 0.78, [alpha] = .71). Representative items appear in the
introduction.
Biblical Foundationalism. The Biblical Foundationalism Scale (M =
2.63, SD = 1.07, [alpha] = .97) included 15 items that ISM procedures
previously identified as reflecting a less defensive commitment to
fundamentals than the Altemeyer and Hunsberger (1992) Religious
Fundamentalism Scale (Watson et al., 2003). One item said, for example,
"The bloodshed of human history makes it clear that evil cannot be
dismissed as the effect merely of 'bad human impulses.' The
reality of evil is captured instead in the biblical depiction of Satan
as the 'Prince of Darkness' who tempts us."
Religious Fundamentalism. Participants responded to the 12-item
Altemeyer and Hunsberger (2004) Religious Fundamentalism Scale (M =
2.37, SD = 0.95, [alpha] = .91). Indicative of this construct was the
reverse scored assertion that "'Satan' is just the name
people give to their own bad impulses. There really is no such thing as
a diabolical 'Prince of Darkness' who tempts us."
Procedure
Student participation in this project was fully voluntary, and all
procedures received institutional approval. Responding to the
questionnaire booklet occurred in a large classroom setting.
Participants entered responses to all items on standardized answer
sheets, which optical scanning equipment later read into a computer data
file. Statistical procedures scored all instruments in terms of the
average response per item. Analyses began with an examination of
correlations among measures. Partial correlations then reexamined
relationships after controlling for Biblical Foundationalism in order to
investigate a Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround and after
controlling Religious Fundamentalism in order to explore a Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surround.
Results
Table 1 reviews correlations among those constructs that were
relevant to religious and psychological openness. Included in these
measures were the Religious Reflection, Religious Schema, and Need for
Cognition scales. These data most importantly demonstrated that Faith
Oriented Reflection displayed the expected negative relationship with
Intellect Oriented Reflection. Most but not all remaining relationships
identified Faith Oriented Reflection and Truth of Texts and Teaching as
relatively closed religious perspectives in contrast to the openness of
the other constructs. Specifically, Faith Oriented Reflection correlated
positively with Truth of Texts and Teaching and negatively with
Xenosophia and Need for Cognition. Truth of Texts and Teachings also
correlated negatively with Xenosophia and Need for Cognition. Positive
linkages with Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality, nevertheless,
suggested that both Faith Oriented Reflection and Truth of Texts and
Teachings had a least some potential for tolerance. In line with the
assumption that it recorded religious openness, Intellect Oriented
Reflection predicted lower scores on Truth of Texts and Teachings and
higher scores on Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality; Xenosophia; and
Need for Cognition. Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality; Xenosophia;
and Need for Cognition all co-varied directly, as would be expected for
presumed indices of openness.
Correlational evidence suggested some differentiation of the
extra-traditional Quest measure from intra-traditional religious
commitments. With one exception, all relationships among the Intrinsic
and Extrinsic Religious Orientations were positive and statistically
significant (M r = .42, SD = .23). These associations ranged from .13 (p
< .05) between Disorder Avoidance and the Extrinsic Social
Orientation to .81 (p < .001) between Family and Social Order and
Disorder Avoidance. The lone exception was a nonsignificant Intrinsic
linkage with the Extrinsic Social motivation (-.09, p = 11). In
contrast, Quest correlated negatively with the Family and Social Order
(-.31), Disorder Avoidance (-.34), Cultural Foundations (-.22),
Intrinsic (-.44), and Extrinsic Personal (-.13, p's < .05)
orientations; positively with Extrinsic Social scores (.19, p <
.001); and nonsignificantly with Peace and Justice (-.05, p = .35).
Relationships with Religious Reflection, Religious Schema, and Need
for Cognition further identified intra-traditional religious commitments
as relatively closed (see Table 2). Faith Oriented Reflection correlated
negatively with Quest and positively with all other religious
orientations. The same pattern appeared for Truth of Texts and Teachings
except that the Extrinsic Social correlation proved to be
nonsignificant. Faith Oriented Reflection and Truth of Texts of
Teaching, therefore, defined intra-traditional perspectives that were
incompatible with an extra-traditional Quest. In contrast, Intellect
Oriented Reflection data suggested that it was compatible with Quest,
Peace and Justice, and the Extrinsic Social orientations, but
incompatible with the Intrinsic, Family and Social Order, Disorder
Avoidance, and Cultural Foundations motivations. Xenosophia correlated
negatively with the Intrinsic Scale and positively with Quest, Peace and
Justice, and the Extrinsic Personal and Social motivations. Need for
Cognition correlated positively with Quest, nonsignificantly with
Extrinsic Social scores, and negatively with all other religious
orientations. The only significant outcome for Fairness, Tolerance and
Rationality was a direct connection with the Extrinsic Personal
Orientation. In short, Intellect Oriented Reflection, Xenosophia, and
Need for Cognition measured an openness that was relatively more
extra-traditional in its implications.
Table 3 presents the centrally important findings of this
investigation. As the ISM makes clear, incommensurable rationalities can
be compatible, and Religious Fundamentalism and Biblical Foundationalism
in fact exhibited a robust positive correlation (.82, p < .001). In
the zero-order correlations reviewed in Table 3, both Religious
Fundamentalism and Biblical Foundationalism displayed linkages
indicative of religious defensiveness, specifically involving negative
correlations with Intellect Oriented Reflection, Need for Cognition,
Xenosophia, and Quest. Extensive connections with religious commitment
seemed evident for both measures in their positive relationships with
Faith Oriented Reflection, Truth of Texts and Teachings, and all but the
Extrinsic Social religious orientations.
Attempts to statistically control for the influence of ideology
produced largely though not wholly expected outcomes. In partial
correlations controlling for Biblical Foundationalism, the supposedly
more defensive Religious Fundamentalist perspective continued to
correlate negatively with Intellect Oriented Reflection, Xenosophia, and
Quest and to correlate positively with Truth of Texts and Teachings and
with the Intrinsic, Family and Social Order, and Disorder Avoidance
religious motivations. On the other hand, unexpected outcomes appeared
in the findings that previously positive zero-order relationships became
nonsignificant with Faith Oriented Reflection, Peace and Justice, and
Cultural Foundations and also became negative with the Extrinsic
Personal factor. The Extrinsic Social relationship also became negative.
Hence, the Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround did display
evidence of defensiveness while also exhibiting an unexpected
diminishment in religious commitments. In the one result not consistent
with this interpretation, the previously negative zero-order linkage
with Need for Cognition became nonsignificant.
Conversely, Biblical Foundationalism appeared as a much more open
religious perspective after partial correlations controlled for
Religious Fundamentalism. Previously negative zero-order relationships
became positive with Intellect Oriented Reflection and Xenosophia and
nonsignificant with Need for Cognition and Quest. The positive
association with Fairness, Tolerance and Rationality became significant,
and Biblical Foundationalism continued to display direct linkages with
Truth of Texts and Teachings and with all intra-traditional measures of
religious motivation.
Further evidence of Religious Fundamentalist defensiveness and
Biblical Foundationalist openness appeared in the partial correlations
among religious and psychological openness measures. Data above the
diagonal in Table 4 describe the Religious Fundamentalist Ideological
Surround, and results for the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological
Surround appear below the diagonal. The relative defensiveness of
Religious Fundamentalism seemed evident in (1) the failure of Faith
Oriented Reflection to predict anything but Truth of Texts and
Teachings, (2) the negative correlation that continued to exist between
Intellect Oriented Reflection and Truth of Texts and Teachings, and (3)
the inverse Xenosophia association with Truth of Texts and Teachings. In
the contrast, the relative openness of Biblical Foundationalism seemed
obvious in (1) positive correlations of Faith Oriented Reflection with
Intellect Oriented Reflection and with all other measures except Need
for Cognition, (2) the removal of the inverse linkage of Intellect
Oriented Reflection with Truth of Texts and Teachings, and (3) the
elimination of the negative tie of Truth of Texts and Teachings with
Xenosophia.
Religious orientation data within both the Religious Fundamentalist
and Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds continued to reveal
at least some differentiation between intra-traditional commitments and
an extra-traditional Quest. Intra-traditional commitments also seemed
more integrated within the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological
Surround, and the Extrinsic Social Orientation once again seemed
ambiguous in its implications. More specifically, within the Religious
Fundamentalist Ideological Surround, partial correlations controlling
for Biblical Foundationalism revealed that the four Extrinsic Cultural
Orientations continued to correlate positively with each other
([r.sub.ab.c] > .38, p < .001). Quest predicted higher Extrinsic
Social (.23) and lower Intrinsic (-.23) and Disorder Avoidance (-.11, ps
< .05) motivations. The Intrinsic Scale only displayed a significant
inverse relationship with the
Extrinsic Social motivation (-.20, p < .01), and Extrinsic
Personal and Social scores correlated positively with each other and
with all four Extrinsic Cultural scales ([r.sub.ab.c] > .20, p <
.001).
Within the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround, partial
correlations controlling for Religious Fundamentalism revealed that
Quest correlated positively with Extrinsic Social (.21, p < .001) and
negatively with Intrinsic (-.13, p < .05) scores. Extrinsic Cultural
measures once again displayed direct linkages ([r.sub.ab.c] > .44, p
< .001). Additional Intrinsic relationships were positive with the
Extrinsic Personal, Family and Social Order, and Cultural Foundations
variables ([r.sub.ab.c] > .11) and negative with Extrinsic Social
scores (.12, p's < .05). Once again, Extrinsic Personal and
Social motivations correlated positively with each other and with the
four Extrinsic Cultural factors ([r.sub.ab.c] > .18, p < .01).
Final evidence of the relative openness and stronger religious
integration of Biblical Foundationalism appeared in partial correlations
of religious orientations with the openness measures (see Table 5). In
contrast to the Religious Fundamentalist data, Biblical Foundationalist
results revealed positive rather than nonsignificant linkages of (1)
Faith Oriented Reflection with the Intrinsic Scale, (2) Truth of Texts
and Teachings with the Extrinsic Personal Orientation, and (3)
Xenosophia with Disorder Avoidance and Family and Social Order. A
nonsignificant rather than negative association also appeared between
the Intrinsic Scale and both Intellect Oriented Reflection and
Xenosophia. Of less conceptual significance was a slight reduction in
the Need for Cognition relationship with Quest that made this
association nonsignificant rather than positive within the Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surround.
Discussion
In exploring the Religious Openness Hypothesis, this investigation
uncovered clear support for two broad sets of predictions. A first
hypothesis essentially suggested that Religious Fundamentalism after
controlling for Biblical Foundationalism would describe the ideological
surround of a more defensive commitment to Christian fundamentals. The
second hypothesis argued that Biblical Foundationalism after controlling
for Religious Fundamentalism would instead define the ideological
surround of a more open commitment to fundamentals. Confirmation of
these two sets of predictions appeared in partial correlations observed
with and for Religious Reflection, Religious Schema, and Religious
Orientation variables.
More specifically, the Religious Fundamentalist Ideological
Surround combined tendencies to reject openness with at least some
commitment to fundamentals. With regard to reduced openness, Religious
Fundamentalism after controlling for Biblical Foundationalism correlated
negatively with Intellect Oriented Reflection and Xenosophia and
displayed no significant connection with Fairness, Tolerance, and
Rationality. The unexpected nonsignificant relationship with Faith
Oriented Reflection suggested an even more defensive ghettoization in
which Christians even failed to bring reflection based upon their faith
into thoughtful contact with experience. The expected linkage with a
commitment to fundamentals seemed obvious in positive partial
correlations of Religious Fundamentalism with Truth of Texts and
Teachings and with the Intrinsic, Family and Social Order, and Disorder
Avoidance Religious Orientations. At the same time, however, religious
commitments seemed at least somewhat diminished because Religious
Fundamentalism correlated negatively with the Extrinsic Personal and the
(admittedly ambiguous) Extrinsic Social Orientations and
nonsignificantly with Peace and Justice and with Cultural Foundations. A
Christian perspective that remained silent about motivations to promote
peace and justice and to influence culture presumably would also point
toward a more ghettoized ideological surround.
Conversely, the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround
combined openness with a commitment to fundamentals. With regard to
openness, Biblical Foundationalism after controlling for Religious
Fundamentalism predicted greater Intellect as well as Faith Oriented
Reflection and also higher levels of Xenosophia and Faith, Tolerance,
and Rationality. With regard to a dedication to fundamentals, Biblical
Foundationalism displayed a positive partial correlation with Truth of
Texts and Teachings, and a relatively more expansive religious
commitment seemed evident in its direct associations with all Intrinsic,
Extrinsic, and Extrinsic Cultural Religious Orientation measures.
Additional Support
Numerous additional findings supported the Religious Openness
Hypothesis. Most importantly, a negative zero-order linkage between the
two forms of religious reflection became positive after partialing out
Religious Fundamentalism, an effect observed previously (Watson et al.,
2011). Within a Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround, Faith
Oriented Reflection failed to predict Xenosophia or Fairness, Tolerance,
and Rationality; and Truth of Texts and Teachings predicted lower levels
of both Intellect Oriented Reflection and Xenosophia. Within a Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surround, however, Faith Oriented Reflection
correlated positively with Xenosophia and with Fairness, Tolerance, and
Rationality; and Truth of Texts and Teachings displayed nonsignificant
rather than negative associations with Intellect Oriented Reflection and
Xenosophia. In short, Religious Reflection and Religious Schema data
further demonstrated that the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological
Surround described a more open and the Religious Fundamentalist
Ideological Surround a less open religious perspective.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Religious Orientation data also generally
supported implications of the Religious Openness Hypothesis. The
Intrinsic Scale correlated positively with Faith Oriented Reflection and
nonsignificantly with Intellect Oriented Reflection within a Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surround, but these relationships became
nonsignificant and negative, respectively, within a Religious
Fundamentalist Ideological Surround. Within the Religious Fundamentalist
Ideological Surround, Xenosophia displayed linkages that were negative
with Intrinsic and nonsignificant with Disorder Avoidance and Family and
Social Order Religious Orientations. Within the Biblical Foundationalist
surround, Xenosophia relationships were instead nonsignificant with the
Intrinsic and positive with these two Extrinsic Cultural scales.
Religious perspectives, therefore, seemed less polarized and more
integrated within the Biblical Foundationalist than within the Religious
Fundamentalist Ideological Surround.
Central to development of the Religious Openness Hypothesis were
concerns about the validity of Quest as a specifically religious form of
openness (e.g., Dover et al., 2007). In the present project as well,
Quest seemed closer to intellect than to faith. This was so because
Quest correlated negatively with Faith Oriented Reflection and Truth of
Texts and Teachings, and positively with Intellect Oriented Reflection
and Need for Cognition. Positive zero-order or partial correlations with
Xenosophia and with Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality further
documented the openness of Quest. Perhaps most importantly, however,
Quest displayed partial correlations that were negative with Religious
Fundamentalism, but nonsignificant with Biblical Foundationalism. This
contrast suggested once again that Biblical Foundationalism was less
defensive than Religious Fundamentalism, a conclusion supported by
another recent examination of Quest (Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014).
Administration of the Need for Cognition Scale made it possible to
evaluate the cognitive openness of religious measures. Along with a
direct linkage with Quest, positive correlations with Xenosophia and
with Fairness, Tolerance and Rationality confirmed the openness of these
constructs. At the same time, Need for Cognition associations with
Religious Fundamentalism, Biblical Foundationalism, and religious
orientations proved to be negative or nonsignificant. Negative
relationships suggested that religious commitments were at least
somewhat incompatible with cognitive openness. Findings that Need for
Cognition partial correlations with both Religious Fundamentalism and
Biblical Foundationalism became nonsignificant in contrast the negative
zero-order relationships, nevertheless, meant that it was unclear how to
interpret these results. The counterintuitive suggestion was that the
negative zero-order relationship was as attributable to the openness of
Biblical Foundationalism as to the defensiveness of Religious
Fundamentalism. The Need for Cognition Scale can also have complex
implications in Indian Hindu samples (Kamble et al., 2014b). Overall,
such outcomes suggest a need to further examine the issue of cognitive
openness and religious commitments in American Christians, perhaps using
a broader array of relevant cognitive measures that might include, for
example, openness to experience.
Broader Implications
In summary, this investigation supported the Religious Openness
Hypothesis with four broader implications perhaps being most noteworthy.
First, the Religious Openness Hypothesis argues that Christianity and
other traditional religions define openness in terms that are compatible
with the standards of their own rationalities. Supporting evidence comes
from demonstrations that Faith and Intellect Oriented Religious
Reflection correlate positively in Iranian Muslims (Ghorbani et al.,
2013) and Indian Hindus (Kamble et al., 2014b). Such relationships
document the ability of traditional religions to unite faith with
intellect. A negative correlation between these two measures in American
Christians may seem to contradict the claim (Watson et al., 2011), but
the Religious Openness Hypothesis explains this effect in terms of a
fundamentalist defensiveness in response to Western secularization.
Defensiveness, in other words, encourages a retreat of Faith Oriented
Reflection into an epistemological ghetto that walls out an Intellect
Oriented Reflection that seems closer to the Enlightenment-based
processes of secularization (Stout, 1988). American commitment to
fundamentals without defensiveness should, therefore, be compatible with
both Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection. Findings for the Biblical
Foundationalist Ideological Surround confirmed that expectation.
Second, fundamentalism in West, therefore, may include an element
of defensiveness that is not evident in other societies like Iran and
India. Among other things, this means that caution seems essential in
drawing inferences about "fundamentalism" world-wide based
upon data from just one society or another. The importance of such
interpretative caution was already evident in a previous demonstration
that an empirical marker of fundamentalism in Iran predicted greater
openness to experience, when the opposite relationship would presumably
be the expectation in the West (Ghorbani, Watson, Shamohammadi, &
Cunningham, 2009).
Third, in a recent historical analysis, Gregory (2012) narrated the
unintended secularizing consequences of the Protestant Reformation and
lamented the broader cultural impact of fundamentalism in the West.
Because of fundamentalism, he argued, "Viewed from the secularist
side from the 'culture wars,' simply to be a religious
believer who actually believes anything of substance is considered
objectionable" (Gregory, p. 356). The present and previous
investigations suggest that deeper understandings of Biblical
Foundationalism and the ISM may be useful in offering non-defensive and
constructive responses to such secularist objections. Biblical
Foundationalist data suggest that Bible-based beliefs can support the
"openness" that is a hallmark of secularism. The ISM emphasis
on incommensurable rationalities also means that faith in "anything
of substance" cannot be a meaningful charge against those with
religious commitments or against anyone else. This is so because
secularists have their own substantive faith in nature as the ultimate
standard (e.g., Connor, Riches, Imfeld, & Hampson, 2012).
Fourth and finally, the Religious Openness Hypothesis appears
useful in generating important research questions. A skeptic, for
example, might argue against the notion that defensiveness explains the
polarization of Western religious reflection and that this relationship
merely documents how Christians are more narrow-minded than Muslims in
Iran and Hindus in India. This skepticism can be tested. The Religious
Openness Hypothesis predicts that Intellect and Faith Oriented
Reflection should correlate positively in Christians living outside the
West where secularization is less culturally influential and where
defensiveness should consequently thus be less evident. This hypothesis
has in fact been tested with the results once again supporting the
Religious Openness Hypothesis (Watson, Ghorbani, Vartanian, & Chen,
2015).
P. J. Watson
University of Tennessee at Chattanooga
Zhuo Chen
University of Oregon
Nima Ghorbani
Meghedi Vartanian
University of Tehran
Address all correspondence to P. J. Watson, Psychology/Department
#2803, 350 Holt Hall--615 McCallie Avenue, University of Tennessee at
Chattanooga 37403, U.S.A. E-mail address is
[email protected]
References
Allport, G. W., & Ross, M. J., Jr. (1967). Personal religious
orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
5, 432-443.
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism,
religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113-133.
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (2004). A revised Religious
Fundamentalism Scale: The short and sweet of it. The International
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14, 47-54.
Batson, C. D., & Schoenrade, P. (1991a). Measuring religion as
quest: 1) Validity concerns. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 30, 416-429.
Batson, C. D., & Schoenrade, P. (1991b). Measuring religion as
quest: 2) Reliability concerns. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 30, 430-447.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion
and the individual. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W.
B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The
life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition.
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197-253.
Connor, C., Riches, A., Imfeld, Z. L., & Hampson, P. (2012) .
Interview and conversation with Conor Cunningham and Aaron Riches:
Radical orthodoxy and Christian psychology II-ontological naturalism and
christology. Edification, 6(1), 69-79.
Donahue, M. J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness:
Review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
48, 400-419.
Dover, H., Miner, M., & Dowson, M. (2007). The nature and
structure of Muslim religious reflection. Journal of Muslim Mental
Health, 2, 189-210.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Dover, H. (2013) .
Varieties of openness in Tehran and Qom: Psychological and religious
parallels of faith and intellect oriented Islamic religious reflection.
Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 16, 123-137.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., & Khan, Z. (2007). Theoretical,
empirical, and potential ideological dimensions of using Western
conceptualizations to measure Muslim religious commitments. Journal of
Muslim Mental Health, 2, 113-131.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Saeedi, Z., Chen, Z., & Silver, C.
F. (2012). Religious problem-solving and the complexity of religious
rationality within an Iranian Muslim ideological surround. Journal for
the Scientific Study of Religion, 51, 656-675.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Shamohammadi, K., & Cunningham, C.
J. L. (2009). Post-critical beliefs in Iran: Predicting religious and
psychological functioning. Research in the Social Scientific Study of
Religion, 20, 217-237.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Zarehi, J., & Shamohammadi, K.
(2010). Muslim extrinsic cultural religious orientation and identity:
Relationships with social and personal adjustment in Iran. Journal of
Beliefs and Values, 31, 15-28.
Gorsuch, R. L., & McPherson, S. E. (1989). Intrinsic/extrinsic
measurement: I/E revised and single-item scales. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 348-354.
Gregory, B. S. (2012). The unintended reformation. Cambridge, MA:
The Belknap Press.
Hood, R. W., Jr., Hill, P. C., & Williamson, W. P. (2005). The
psychology of religious fundamentalism. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kamble, S. V., Watson, P. J., Marigoudar, S., & Chen, Z.
(2014a). Attitude toward Hinduism, religious orientations, and
psychological adjustment in India. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture,
17, 161-172.
Kamble, S. V., Watson, P. J., Marigoudar, S., & Chen, Z.
(2014b). Varieties of openness and religious commitment in India:
Relationships of attitudes toward Hinduism, Hindu religious reflection,
and religious schema. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 36,
172-198.
MacIntyre, A. (1978). Against the self-images of the age. Notre
Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1988). Whose justice? Which rationality? Notre Dame,
IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
Streib, H., Hood, R. W, Jr., & Klein, C. (2010). The Religious
Schema Scale: Construction and initial validation of a quantitative
measure for religious styles. International Journal for the Psychology
of Religion, 20, 151-172.
Stout, J. (1988). Ethics after Babel. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Watson, P. J. (1993). Apologetics and ethnocentrism: Psychology and
religion within an ideological surround. International Journal for the
Psychology of Religion, 3, 1-20.
Watson, P. J. (2011). Whose psychology? Which rationality?
Christian psychology within an ideological surround after postmodernism.
Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 30, 307-316.
Watson, P. J. (2014). Transition beyond post-modernism: Pluralistic
culture, incommensurable rationalities, and future objectivity. Review
& Expositor, 111, 33-40.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Ghorbani, N. (2014). Extrinsic
cultural religious orientation: Analysis of an Iranian measure in
university students in the United States. Journal of Beliefs and Values,
35, 61-78.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (2011). Biblical
foundationalism and religious reflection: Polarization of faith and
intellect oriented epistemologies within a Christian ideological
surround. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 39, 111-121.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Morris, R. J. (2014). Varieties of
quest and the religious openness hypothesis within religious
fundamentalist and biblical foundationalist ideological surrounds.
Religions, 5(1), 1-20.
Watson, P. J., Ghorbani, N., Vartanian, M., & Chen, Z. (2015).
Religious openness hypothesis: II. Religious reflection and
orientations, mystical experience, and psychological openness of
Christians in Iran. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 34, 114-124.
Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (1987).
Antireligious humanistic values, guilt, and self-esteem. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 26, 535-546.
Watson, P. J., Sawyers, P., Morris, R. J., Carpenter, M., Jimenez,
R. S., Jonas, K. A., & Robinson, D. L. (2003). Reanalysis within a
Christian ideological surround: Relationships of intrinsic religious
orientation with fundamentalism and right-wing authoritarianism. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 31, 315-328.
Authors
P.J. Watson is U. C. Foundation Professor of Psychology at the
University of Tennessee at Chatttanooga. He received a Ph.D. in
Experimental Psychology from the University of Texas at Arlington. His
research focuses on the psychology of religion and on personality
functioning especially as it relates to the self.
Zhuo Chen is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Psychology
at the University of Oregon. His research area is in personality and
social psychology. He received a master's degree in Research
Psychology at the University of Tennessee at Chattanooga and also a
master's degree in Mathematics at the University of Oregon.
Nima Ghorbani, PhD, is a psychologist with the University of Tehran
in Iran. He is a licensed practitioner of Intensive Short-Term Dynamic
Psychotherapy. His reserach interests include cross-cultural
perspectives on the self and emotion and also on the psychology of the
religious self and experience.
Meghedi Vartanian is a graduate student studying psychology at the
University of Tehran in Iran. Her research interests center on the self,
psychotherapy, and the psychology of religion.
Table 1
Correlations Among Religious Reflection, Religious
Schema, and Need for Cognition Scales
(N = 350)
Measures 1 2 3
1. Faith Oriented Reflection - -.18 ** .72 ***
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection - - -.34 ***
3. Truth of Texts and Teaching - - -
4. Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality - - -
5. Xenosophia - - -
6. Need for Cognition - - -
Measures 4 5 6
1. Faith Oriented Reflection .11 ** -.16 ** -.26 ***
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection .26 *** .38 *** .25 ***
3. Truth of Texts and Teaching .18 ** -.29 *** -.24 ***
4. Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality - .28 *** .19 ***
5. Xenosophia - - .22 ***
6. Need for Cognition - - -
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 2
Correlations of Religious Reflection, Religious Schema, and Need
for Cognition Scales With Religious Orientations (N = 350)
Reflection, Schema, and
Need for Cognition Scales
Religious Orientations FOR IOR TTT
Intrinsic .69 *** -.36 *** .82 ***
Extrinsic Personal .53 *** .06 .40 ***
Extrinsic Social .11 * .12 * 0.02
Family and Social Order .68 *** -.21 *** .64 ***
Disorder Avoidance .66 *** -.24 *** .64 ***
Peace and Justice .39 *** .12 * .22 ***
Cultural Foundations .58 *** -.12 * .54 ***
Quest -.38 *** .40 *** -.48 ***
Reflection, Schema, and
Need for Cognition Scales
Religious Orientations FTR Xen NfC
Intrinsic .10 -.28 *** -.20 ***
Extrinsic Personal .16 ** .16 ** -.14 **
Extrinsic Social .06 .12 * -.08
Family and Social Order .03 -.09 -.32 ***
Disorder Avoidance .01 -.09 -.29 ***
Peace and Justice .08 .18 ** -.11 *
Cultural Foundations .08 -.03 -.19 ***
Quest .09 .45 *** .21 ***
Note. Scales include Faith Oriented Reflection (FOR), Intellect
Oriented Reflection (IOR), Truth of Texts and Teachings (TTT),
Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality (FTR), Xenosophia (Xen),
and Need for Cognition (NfC).
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 3
Zero-Order (r) and Partial (rab.c) Correlations of Religious
Fundamentalism and Biblical Foundationalism With Other
Measures (N = 350)
Variable Religious Fundamentalism
r rab.c
Faith Oriented Reflection .77 *** .10
Intellect Oriented Reflection -.39 *** -.34 ***
Truth of Texts and Teachings .83 *** .44 ***
Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality .04 -.10
Xenosophia -.64 *** -.37 ***
Need for Cognition -.27 *** -.10
Intrinsic .79 *** .35 ***
Extrinsic Personal .35 *** -.16 **
Extrinsic Social -.02 -.12 *
Family and Social Order .64 *** .18 ***
Disorder Avoidance .65 *** .16 **
Peace and Justice .23 *** -.09
Cultural Foundations .51 *** .03
Quest -.47 *** -.29 ***
Variable Biblical Foundationalism
r rab.c
Faith Oriented Reflection .85 *** .57 ***
Intellect Oriented Reflection -.26 *** .18 **
Truth of Texts and Teachings .80 *** .28 ***
Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality .10 .14 *
Xenosophia -.21 *** .23 ***
Need for Cognition -.26 *** -.05
Intrinsic .78 *** .32 ***
Extrinsic Personal .47 *** .38 ***
Extrinsic Social .05 .13 *
Family and Social Order .66 *** .27 ***
Disorder Avoidance .67 *** .30 ***
Peace and Justice .31 *** .23 ***
Cultural Foundations .57 *** .30 ***
Quest -.40 *** .04
Note: Partial Correlations for Fundamentalism control
for Biblical Foundationalism whereas partial correlations
for Biblical Foundationalism control for Fundamentalism.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 4
Partial Correlations Among Openness Measures Within Religious
Fundamentalist (above diagonal) and Biblical Foundationalist
(below diagonal) Ideological Surrounds (N = 350)
Measures 1 2 3
1. Faith Oriented Reflection - .09 .14 *
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection .20 *** - -.23 ***
3. Truth of Texts and Teaching .24 *** -.04 -
4. Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality .12 * .30 *** .25 ***
5. Xenosophia .19 *** .28 *** .02
6. Need for Cognition -.08 .16 ** -.03
Measures 4 5 6
1. Faith Oriented Reflection .04 .02 -.07
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection .30 *** .34 *** .20 ***
3. Truth of Texts and Teaching .16 ** -.21 *** -.05
4. Fairness, Tolerance, Rationality - .31 *** .23 ***
5. Xenosophia .31 *** - .18 **
6. Need for Cognition .21 *** .14 * -
Note. Partial correlations controlling for Biblical Foundationalism
define a Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround, whereas
partial correlations controlling for Religious Fundamentalism
define a Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 5
Thoughtful Seeking Partial Cotrelations with Religious Orientations
Within Religious Fundamentalist and Biblical Foundationalist
Ideological Surrounds (N = 350)
Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround
Measure FOR IOR TTT
Intrinsic .07 -.26 *** .51 ***
Extrinsic Personal .27 *** .21 *** .03
Extrinsic Social .13 * .13 * -.04
Family/Sociai Order .30 *** -.05 .25 ***
Disorder Avoidance .23 *** -.09 .24 ***
Peace and Justice .25 *** .22 *** -.04
Cultural Foundations .22 *** .03 .18 **
Quest -.09 .33 *** -.29 ***
Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround
Measure FTR Xen NfG
Intrinsic .03 -.19 *** .00
Extrinsic Personal .13 ** .30 *** -.03
Extrinsic Social .06 .13 * -.08
Family/Sociai Order -.06 .07 -.20 ***
Disorder Avoidance -.09 .07 -.16 **
Peace and Justice .06 .26 *** -.04
Cultural Foundations .03 .11 * -.05
Quest .14 ** .41 *** .12 *
Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround
Measure FOR IOR TTT
Intrinsic .21 *** -.09 .47 ***
Extrinsic Personal .44 *** .22 *** .21 ***
Extrinsic Social .19 *** .12 * .06
Family/Sociai Order .38 *** .06 .25 ***
Disorder Avoidance .34 *** .01 .26 ***
Peace and Justice .34 *** .23 *** .07
Cultural Foundations .34 *** .10 .26 ***
Quest -.03 .26 *** -.17 **
Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround
Measure FTR Xen NfG
Intrinsic .10 .01 .02
Extrinsic Personal .15 * 32 *** -.06
Extrinsic Social .06 .12 * -.09
Family/Sociai Order .00 .21 *** -.19 ***
Disorder Avoidance -.03 .20 *** -.16 **
Peace and Justice .08 .29 *** -.06
Cultural Foundations .07 .19 *** -.06
Quest .12 * .35 *** .10
Note. The Religious Fundamentalist Ideological Surround reflects
partial correlations controlling for Biblical Foundational ism,
whereas the Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surround involves
partial correlations controlling for Religious Fundamentalism.
Measures include Faith Oriented Reflection (FOR), Intellect
Oriented Reflection (IOR), Truth of Texts and Teachings (TTT),
Fairness, Tolerance, and Rationality (FTR), Xenosophia (Xen), and
Need for Cognition (NfC).
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001