Acculturation of Korean multicultural Christian college students.
Cook, Kaye V. ; Sim, DongGun
Acculturation is the "phenomenon which results when groups of
individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand
contact," with subsequent changes primarily in the cultural
patterns of one group (Berry, 1997, p. 7). Acculturation is a
multifaceted concept, and one may adapt in some areas but not in others.
True assimilation (the highest level of acculturation, defined as
rejection of one's birth culture and adoption of the cultural norms
of the dominant culture, Berry, 1997) may never occur (Trimble, 2003).
Most studies of individual acculturation have not examined the
acculturation of values (Kim, Yang, Atkinson, Wolfe, & Hong, 2001;
Marin & Gamba, 2003), instead exploring mental or physical health,
but the acculturation of values, despite the challenges of developing
adequate measures, deserves further attention. The current study uses
qualitative and quantitative data to explore the acculturation of Korean
college students into an American Christian college, with attention to
their cultural patterns (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998), the ethics that
participants express (Shweder, Much, Mahapatra, & Park, 1997), and
the values that they hold (Schwartz, 1992, 1994, 2011).
In a widely known description of two basic psychological domains by
which cultures can be described, Triandis and Gelfand (1998) identified
two major types of cultural patterns: individualism and collectivism.
Individualism refers to cultures which value a personal definition of
the self, give personal goals priority, emphasize exchange in
relationships, and emphasize rationality rather than relatedness,
whereas collectivist cultures value in-group goals and an interdependent
sense of self. These two basic cultural patterns provide a way for
thinking about the distinctive values of the American and Korean
cultures.
Further, within both individualistic and collectivistic cultures,
Triandis and Gelfand (1998) differentiate between horizontal and
vertical patterns of the culture, depending on the dominant social
values in the culture. In cultures with horizontal patterns, individuals
value equality and assum that one self is more or less like every other
self; in vertical pattern, hierarchy is valued and "one self is
different from other selves" (p. 119). These horizontal and
vertical patterns--with their different emphases on equality and
hierarchy--combine with individualism and collectivism to produce four
distinct patterns by which some components of cultural values can be
captured: horizontal individualism, vertical individualism, horizontal
collectivism, and vertical collectivism. Using these four categories,
Korea has been identified as a culture which values horizontal
individualism and collectivism equally, whereas the United States has
been identified as a horizontal individualist culture (Triandis &
Gelfland, 1998). Despite the acculturation of Koreans to American
culture, we expected to find that Koreans retain some of the cultural
patterns of Korea and therefore that they would show more collectivist
responses to scenarios.
In addition, this study explores the acculturation of Korean
students by examining participant ethics and self-expressed values,
based on their responses to two questions about important values in
one's own life and for the next generation. Ethics were coded into
one of three categories defined by Shweder et al. (1997): Autonomy,
Community and Divinity. According to Arnett, Ramos, and Jensen (2001, p.
7), the ethic of Autonomy "defines the individual as the primary
moral authority, unrestricted in choices except by his or her own
preferences" whereas the ethic of Community describes individuals
as "members of groups to which they have commitments and
obligations." In comparison, "an ethic of Divinity includes
beliefs and values based on traditional religious authorities and
sometimes on religious texts such as the Bible and the Koran"
(2001, p. 70). We expected to find that Korean students produced ethics
of Community more frequently because of their more strongly collectivist
tradition (Sasaki & Kim, 2011).
Because ethics describe values on a cultural and not individual
level, values were also coded using the universal values identified by
Schwartz (1992, 1994, 2011). Values are "desirable transsituational
goals, varying in importance, that serve as guiding principles in the
life of a person or other social entity" (Schwartz, 1994, p. 21).
Values originate from culture and society and are broad cultural
principles that direct individuals' behavior. Through a series of
studies encompassing more than 40 countries, Schwartz identified 10
basic types of human values--power, achievement, hedonism, stimulation,
self-direction, universalism, benevolence, tradition, conformity, and
security. The degree to individuals adopt these values is influenced by
their distinctive cultures. Schwartz proposed, for example, that the
value of self-direction is related to individualism and the values of
tradition and conformity are linked to collectivism (Schwartz, 1992).
Schwartz described and defined a few of these values as follows.
Individuals who are self-directed are independent in thinking, acting,
creating, and exploring, and individuals who espouse universalism as a
value are concerned with understanding, appreciation, tolerance, and
general concern for others' benevolence. Individuals who value
tradition are humble, moderate, and accept traditional cultural customs.
Individuals who value conformity exercise restraint and are unlikely to
violate social norms. Individuals who value security focus on safety,
harmony, stability, and a sense of belonging. Further, whereas
universalism reflects a general concern for others, is more individual
in attempts to preserve and enhance the welfare of personally known
others (Workman & Lee, 2011).
It is noteworthy that, in an earlier study using the Schwartz Value
Scale (Cook, Sandage, Hill, & Strawn, 2010), individuals from
different cultures were found to differ in some of the values they hold
but also to hold the same values but in somewhat different ways. For
example, Cambodian immigrants differed from American nonimmigrants in
valuing duty as a motivation for virtue, in recognizing the collectivist
impact of practicing virtues, and in valuing balance and moderation when
practicing virtue. In a separate study, Sandage, Hill, and Vang (2003)
found that, in contrast to the meaning of forgiveness in American
culture, forgiveness was understood in the context of social face,
respect, and a history of trauma for one Hmong immigrant couple. We
predict that Koreans will express more values that reflect their birth
culture's emphasis on collectivist patterns, including tradition
and security. Americans will use more of the individualist values of and
self-direction than Koreans (Schwartz, 1992).
Several studies have shown that the formation of personal beliefs
and values is a crucial part of becoming acculturated (Greene, Wheatley,
& Aldava, 1992; Marin & Gamba, 2003); however, values are often
not studied in the acculturation literature (Kim et al., 2001; Marin
& Gamba, 2003). We offer these data, both qualitative and
quantitative, in an effort to better understand the process of
acculturation for values.
Method
Participants
Fifty individuals--twenty-five bilingual Koreans and twenty-five
monolingual Americans--were recruited from among undergraduate students
at a small Christian liberal arts college in the northeast which has
approximately 1500 students, 2.1% of which are Korean international
students. Among the 32 Korean students who were approached, 25 agreed to
participate: 14 freshmen, 3 sophomores, 4 juniors, and 4 seniors. The
American sample was designed to match the Korean sample, and thus
similarly had 25 participants of which 14 were freshmen, 3 were
sophomores, 4 were juniors, and 4 were seniors, with gender
distributions that paralleled the Korean sample (18 males and 7
females). Although the bilingual students were ethnically Korean, the
majority of them spent an extended period--more than a year--of their
lives in one or more cultures other than their own including China, the
Philippines, Malaysia, Taiwan, Cambodia, Singapore, Sri Lanka, Russia,
Uzbekistan, Kazakhstan, and the United States. None of the American
students had lived outside the States more than a year. For this reason,
although the participants were selected to be monolingual or bilingual,
they are also described in this paper as monocultural and multicultural.
Procedure
The names of bilingual Korean students were obtained from the
on-campus international organization and individuals were contacted by
email and asked to participate. A comparison group of American students,
matched by gender and class, was recruited from psychology classes and
received extra credits for their participation.
If the individual agreed to participate, confidentiality was
explained and they were given a questionnaire to complete that took
about 10 minutes. They then were asked two questions, with follow-up
questions as needed to clarify. One researcher individually met each
participant and administered the questionnaire and the interview, both
in English. The interview was audiotaped and transcribed. Administration
of the survey and interview took less than 30 minutes. No participant
received financial remuneration for participating.
Measures
Participants first completed the 16 Scenarios for the Measurement
of Collectivism and Individualism (which measures horizontal and
vertical individualism and collectivism; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998).
For each scenario, a two-sentence description of a real-life scenario
(e.g., you and your friends decided spontaneously to go out to dinner at
a restaurant. What do you think is the best way to handle the bill?) is
followed by four options. Each of the choices in the question
corresponds to one of the four cultural categories--horizontal
individualism (HI), horizontal collectivism (HC), vertical individualism
(VI), and vertical collectivism (VC). Summarizing a participant's
responses allows identifying the respondents' cultural pattern.
Triandis and Gelfand (1998) suggested that, using this scale, the
cultural profile of the United States can be expressed as HI > HC
> VI > VC whereas the cultural profile of Korea can be expressed
as HI = HC > VI > VC.
Two questions from Arnett et al. (2001) were used to measure
participants' ethics and individual values: a) when you get toward
the end of your life, what would you like to be able to say about your
life, looking back on it? and b) what values or beliefs do you think are
the most important to pass on to the next generation? For 11 bilingual
Korean students, the interviewer asked an additional question: how have
your values or beliefs changed since coming to America? Responses to the
two questions asked of all participants and to the additional question
asked of 11 Koreans were coded into one of the three categories of
Autonomy, Community and Divinity (Shweder et al., 1997).
The Shweder categories were coded using criteria recommended by
Arnett et al. (2001). A response that focused on the person's own
needs, desires, and interests was coded as Autonomy. These responses
included statements regarding individual accomplishments (e.g.,
"I'd like to be able to say that I achieved all that I set out
to do"), or personal experiences (e.g., "Just that I had fun,
took a lot of nice vacations"), or statements that asserted
autonomy-related virtues (e.g., "I'd tell them to be true to
yourself and to have respect for yourself"). A response that
focused on the needs, desires, and interests of others was coded as
Community. These responses included statements related to family (e.g.,
"That I was a good father to my kids") and other specific
persons or groups (e.g., "To be a faithful and loyal friend")
as well as more general statements of consideration for others (e.g.,
"Being good to the people around you"). A response that
focused on religious authorities, religious texts, or religious beliefs
was coded as Divinity, including references to specific religious
traditions (e.g., "Take time to sit down and read the Bible")
as well as more general statements of religious beliefs (e.g.,
"Just to believe in God"). Consistent with Arnett et al.
(2001), the coding of responses was not mutually exclusive (p. 5).
The interviews were also coded for values using Schwartz'
value scales (1992, 1994). Schwartz, in deriving his list of ten types
of universal values (including Achievement, Benevolence, Conformity,
Security, and Tradition), identified a list of 58 values which together
make up the 10 types of values. For example, benevolence included seven
values (being helpful, honest, forgiving, loyal, and responsible, and
showing true friendship and mature love), and security included family
security, social order, and reciprocation of favors. No coding system
for interviews has been developed and we therefore used thematic coding,
at the recommendation of Schwartz (personal communication, June 26,
2012).
Two coders--a bicultural Korean and a mono-cultural American--read
each interview and independently identified the values that were
expressed. The inter-coder agreement was 92% for Shweder's ethics
(range 66% to 100%), and 85% (range 57% to 100%) for Schwartz types of
values. The agreement for Schwartz single values was 71% (range 60% to
100%). Any disagreements were discussed and resolved, and a coding
manual of phrases that were coded in each value was developed. (1)
Differences in responses to the 16 Scenarios were analyzed by a
Repeated Measures Multivariate of Variance, and differences in ethics
and in individual values were analyzed by Chi-Square Test, where
possible.
Results
Cultural Pattern
Cultural patterns differed by ethnic group such that, as predicted,
the Koreans produced more hierarchical collectivist responses to
scenarios. The linear interaction between cultural pattern and ethnic
group was significant with moderate effect, F(1, 48) = 6.62, p = .01,
[[eta].sup.2] = .12. Specifically, the Koreans had lower scores on the
horizontal individualism scale and higher scores on the vertical scales
than the Americans. (See Table 1.)
Shweder's Ethics
Contrary to prediction, no significant difference was found between
Americans and bilingual Korean students in their use of the three ethics
identified by Shweder et al. (1997). Multicultural Koreans used all
three ethics in roughly equal proportions: Autonomy (28.4%), Community
(37.9%) and Divinity (33.7%), as did the Americans (Autonomy, 26.8%;
Community, 42.2%; Divinity, 30.1%).
Schwartz' Values
Analysis of the values used by bilingual Koreans and monocultural
Americans indicated that Americans favored the values of benevolence
([chi square] (1, N = 50) = 7.61, p < 0.01) whereas bilingual Koreans
favored security ([chi square] [1, N = 50] = 8.53, p < 0.01). The
value of tradition appeared frequently in the responses of both groups.
Tallying the individual responses that were coded "tradition",
most of which were coded "devout", showed that American
students talked about being devout as important for oneself in
developing one's faith (e.g., " ... I grew spiritually and I
really gave God my all and fulfilled His plan for me in my life ...
"). Multicultural Koreans, who also produced many
"devout" responses, many of which were similar to those
produced by the Americans, also talked about the importance of being
devout for the sake of its effects on community ("my passion is for
people in leading them to God's kingdom" and "the
willingness to sacrifice yourself ... I want to teach at a Christian
missionary school"), as a gift from family, and as a responsibility
("the first thing would be faith and that's been passed down
to me from my parents ... my mom always emphasizes the importance of
passing it down to the next generation"). Thus, 15 comments (of 33
from the Koreans) and only four (of 26 from the Americans) emphasized
the importance of being devout for these sorts of communal reasons, [chi
square] (1, N = 59) = 6.01, p < .05.
In response to the added question for 11 Koreans ("how have
your values or beliefs changed since coming to America?"), Korean
students seemed to appreciate their increasing acculturation into
American culture while also valuing their retention of elements of their
birth culture. Nine of 11 multicultural Korean students mentioned that
their values of universalism and benevolence became better developed.
Two people said they were more able to express themselves. On the other
hand, two students responded that their traditional beliefs such as the
value of family and home and the importance of established rules and
guidelines ("submission to authority to a reasonable extent")
had been strengthened and refined.
Discussion
The multicultural Koreans who participated in this study have been
in an American Christian college for at least a semester and were
therefore increasingly acculturated to their context. They nevertheless
retained characteristics of their birth country, including a bias away
from horizontal individualism and towards more verticality in
relationships. Further, when individual values were explored, they
emphasized security and did not value benevolence as much as the
Americans in the study. Despite referencing tradition equally as much as
the Americans, their understanding of tradition appeared to be more
grounded in collectivist values. When asked how they perceive their
values or beliefs to have changed since coming to America, they
perceived themselves as being open to change, as moving toward
benevolence and universalism, and yet as coming to better understandings
of their own traditional beliefs. In other words, these Koreans retained
elements of their cultural tradition while integrating elements of
American culture.
The distinctives between the two groups give us insights into their
different cultural experiences. It is likely that the Koreans often
experience tension between their ethnic culture and the American
culture. A Korean student said, "I am in America currently and
exposed to the culture in that the value of ... wastefulness is a big
issue that I see in our generation ... the second thing is the value of
discipline ... it's very lacking and [this is] becoming
standardized and justified." In addition, one Korean said that his
thinking has the blended elements of two cultures, "I am always
having to think between two cultures and I [was] only [in] Korea for 12
years so I can't really know the full system ... just always trying
to get the best of the two different thinking systems." Their
pattern of acculturation appears better described as integration than as
pure assimilation, separation, or marginalization (Berry, 2003).
Korean students participate in the same mainstream American culture
as Americans and are bilingual, but they are not purely assimilated.
Instead, Korean students may tend to experience American culture as more
hierarchical than Americans. Thus, whereas Koreans responded to
scenarios with more hierarchical and collectivist patterns, American
students highlighted more horizontal individualist patterns. In these
ways, despite values that show their willingness to assimilate and their
openness to assimilation to the American culture, they retain cultural
influences and do not totally assimilate.
Although there were no differences in the ethics (Autonomy,
Community, Divinity) that Koreans and Americans used to respond to the
two questions, exploration of individual values showed that Koreans and
Americans may capture different ideas by similar expressions, as has
been shown in other immigrant groups (Cook et al., 2010). For example,
as shown in their self-expressed values of tradition, American students
talked about tradition as a personal expression of their faith (e.g.,
being devout as "obeying God's commandments") whereas
bilingual Koreans emphasized the communal components of valuing
tradition and being devout (e.g., as being devout for the sake of the
community or honoring one's parents). One bilingual Korean
described her life in this way: "whatever you do, do it ultimately
for others and for God."
Similarities were nevertheless also extensive. Contrary to
prediction, when ethics were explored, Koreans and Americans showed
comparable levels of Autonomy and Community. Our failure to find a
difference between our two groups of participants in these areas may be
because Community and Divinity are highly correlated (r = .46, p <
.01; Guerra & Giner-Sorolla, 2010), and the current sample showed
high levels of Divinity. One-third of Koreans and almost as many
Americans in the current study produced an ethic of Divinity, higher
levels of Divinity ethics than in the culture as a whole (20%; Arnett et
al., 2001). We thus speculate that their Christian beliefs account for
their lack of difference in these measures.
Further, in both groups, many of the descriptions of the values
were similar. For example, both described the devout in terms of
one's relationship to God, e.g., "glorifying God,"
"preparing to meet with the Christ," and "committed to
the Christ," rather than through specific practices of faith such
as prayer or reading the Bible. These similarities in their expressions
of faith surely represent a complex pattern of continuity in their
cultural and faith identities as well as integration and partial
assimilation into a new culture.
This study has significant limitations. We would like to have
interviewed more Koreans, and have had equal numbers of males and
females and of participants at each year. We would also like to have
interviewed Koreans who have just arrived, to get a better sense of how
much these Korean participants might have changed in their values and
ethics. Also, we used the same interview samples to code both ethics and
individual values, and would like to have had a more extended interview
from which to code these qualitative data. The same analyses could have
been done with other ethnic groups, including ones that are biased
toward verticalism rather than horizontalism (as both groups were,
though to differing extents, in our study).
We have tried, however, to provide qualitative and quantitative
data on the acculturation process in value development. We have
documented that, despite apparently successful and extensive
cross-cultural adaptation, our Korean participants retain elements of
their earlier enculturation, including respect for tradition, a desire
for security, and a lesser emphasis on benevolence. They also emphasize
verticalism more and horizontal individualism less than Americans. At
the same time, these individuals adapt elements of their larger
religious, educational and cultural contexts, sharing many of their
values with Americans in a pattern that appears best described as
integration rather than separation or marginalization, or complete
assimilation. Despite the assumptions of many that Koreans easily
assimilate, adopting the characteristics of their host country, these
data suggest that their earlier cultural experiences continue to shape
their values in their new culture.
References
Arnett, J. J., Ramos, K. D., & Jensen, L. A. (2001).
Ideological views in emerging adulthood: Balancing autonomy and
community. Journal of Adult Development, 8, 69-79.
Berry, J. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 5-68.
Berry, J. (2003). Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In K. M.
Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Marin (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in
theory, measurement, and applied research (pp. 17-37). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Cook, K. V., Sandage, S. J., Hill, P. C., & Strawn, B. D.
(2010). Folk conceptions of virtue among Cambodian-American Buddhists
and Christians: A hermeneutic analysis. Psychology of Religion and
Spirituality, 2, 83-103.
Greene, A. L., Wheatley, S. M., & Aldava, J. F. (1992). Stages
on life's way: Adolescents' implicit theories of the life
course. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 364-381.
Guerra, V. M., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2010). The Community,
Autonomy, and Divinity Scale (CADS): A new tool for the cross-cultural
study of morality. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 35-50.
Kim, B. S. K., Yang, P. H., Atkinson, D. R., Wolfe, M. M., &
Hong, S. (2001). Cultural value similarities and differences among Asian
American ethnic groups. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
Psychology, 7, 343-361.
Marin, G., & Gamba, R. J. (2003). Acculturation and changes in
cultural values. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Marin (Eds.),
Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research
(pp. 83-94). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sandage, S. J., Hill, P. C., & Vang, C. (2003). Toward a
multicultural positive psychology: Indigenous forgiveness and Hmong
culture. The Counseling Psychologist, 31, 564-592.
Sasaki, J. Y., & Kim, H. S. (2011). At the intersection of
culture and religion: A cultural analysis of religion's
implications for secondary control and social affiliation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 401-414.
Schwartz, S. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of
values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology: Vol. 25 (pp.
1-24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism: New
cultural dimensions of values. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C.
Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and
Collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 85-119). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Schwartz, S. H. (2011). Values: Cultural and individual. In F. J.
R. Van de Vijver, A. Chasiotis, & S. M. Breugelmans (Eds.),
Fundamental questions in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 463-493). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R., Much, N., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The
"big three" of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and
the "big three" explanations of suffering. In A. Brandt &
P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119-169). London, England:
Routledge.
Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging
measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118-128.
Trimble, J. E. (2003). Introduction: Social change and
acculturation. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Marin (Eds.),
Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Workman, J. E, & Lee, S. H. (2011). Materialism, fashion
consumers and gender: A cross cultural study. International Journal of
Consumer Studies, 35, 5057.
Kaye V Cook
DongGun Sim
Gordon College
Note
(1) Available from the second author.
Authors
Kaye Cook, Ph.D., is Professor of Psychology at Gordon College,
Wenham, MA, where she specializes in teaching courses in developmental
psychology, spiritual formation, and faith-learning integration. Her
primary research interests are in the areas of intercultural and
developmental research on religiosity.
DongGun Sim, B.A., is a recent graduate from Gordon College,
Wenham, MA, where he studied psychology as his major. His experience as
an international student in the states has inspired much of his interest
in the areas of moral development, bilingualism, and intercultural
studies.
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Kaye V.
Cook, Ph.D., Department of Psychology, Gordon College, 255 Grapevine
Rd., Wenham, MA 01984;
[email protected]
Table 1
Four Types of Cultural Patterns by Ethnic Groups
95% Confidence
Interval
Cultural Pattern Ethnic N Mean Std. Lower Upper
Group Error Bound Bound
Horizontal Korean 25 6.12 .40 5.32 6.92
Individualism American 25 6.68 .40 5.88 7.48
Horizontal Korean 25 5.00 .28 4.45 5.55
Collectivism American 25 5.28 .28 4.73 5.83
Vertical Korean 25 2.56 .27 2.01 3.11
Individualism American 25 1.88 .27 1.33 2.43
Vertical Korean 25 2.24 .29 1.66 2.82
Collectivism American 25 2.12 .29 1.54 2.70